956 Child Development
for higher-status out-groups (Tajfel, 1978). This the-
ory is the basis of a recent experimental study by Yee
and Brown (1992), in which children aged 3, 5, 7, and
9 years were assigned to teams, and team status was
manipulated to be high or low. Before the manipula-
tion, children in every age group showed a prefer-
ence for their own team, that is, in-group bias. How-
ever, when some teams were shown to be of lower
status (i.e., losing a race), most children on a "slow"
team chose to change to a "fast" team. Thus, when
their group was seen in an unfavorable light, these
children showed out-group bias. By implication, mi-
nority children whose ethnic group is negatively ste-
reotyped may be subject to out-group bias. In addi-
tion to research with children, there is evidence that
some minority adolescents hold out-group biases, ex-
pressed as a preference for "being White" (Phinney,
1989).
However, out-group bias appears to change with
development. Although some minority children may
hold negative in-group attitudes, these attitudes are
likely to decline during adolescence. As part of the
identity formation process described by Erikson
(1968), ethnic youth explore the meaning and impli-
cations of their ethnic or racial group membership
(Cross, 1991; Helms, 1990; Phinney, 1989, 1990, 1993).
During this period of exploration of, or immersion
in, their culture, adolescents' attitudes toward their
own and other groups may be ambivalent and unsta-
ble. But eventually this period gives way to a secure
and committed sense of self as a group member. This
secure sense of self has been termed an achieved eth-
nic identity and is associated with positive attitudes
toward one's own group (Atkinson, Morten, & Sue,
1993; Cross, 1991; Phinney, 1993; Phinney & Chavira,
1992). In the present study, we used a measure of eth-
nic identity (Phinney, 1992) that includes items as-
sessing ethnic identity achievement (exploration and
commitment) as well as attitudes toward one's
group. On the basis of the developmental research,
we expected that older (eleventh-grade) students
would have a stronger ethnic identity than younger
(eighth-grade) students and, as a corollary, more pos-
itive in-group attitudes.
Furthermore, contextual variables influence out-
group attitudes. Much of the research outside of the
laboratory has focused on the Black-White relations,
usually in settings where Blacks are a subordinate
group (Schofield, 1986). In the present study, we used
schools in which the student body consisted primar-
ily of two minority groups in approximately equal
numbers, to minimize the effects of status differential
based either on the proportion of one's group in the
setting or on the generally higher status of White par-
ticipants. Therefore, there was little reason to expect
participants to prefer another group over their own.
A question that remains unclear, however, is the
relation between in-group and out-group attitudes.
The literature presents contrasting views of this rela-
tion. Early studies of ethnocentrism with adults sug-
gested that more positive in-group attitudes were re-
lated to more negative out-group attitudes. Levinson
(1950) found a strong negative relation between pa-
triotism and attitudes toward foreigners. This view
has received some support in a recent study with
Dutch adolescents (Masson & Verkuyten, 1993),
which found that positive evaluation of one's own
ethnicity was strongly correlated with prejudice to-
ward people who were foreign or different from one-
self.
A second view, from social psychology, holds that
the existence of in-group bias does not necessarily
mean that there is a negative relation between these
attitudes (Messick & Mackie, 1989; Tajfel & Turner,
1986). On the basis of a review of a large number of
studies of intergroup attitudes, Hinkle and Brown
(1990) reported that in-group and out-group atti-
tudes may be positively or negatively related or unre-
lated depending on the conditions. They note, fur-
ther, that much of the research on this topic is based
on homogeneous samples in laboratory settings, and
findings may not apply to settings outside the labora-
tory.
A third view, that in-group and out-group atti-
tudes are positively related, is supported by both de-
velopmental and multiculturalism approaches. The
developmental view (Cross, 1991; Helms, 1990; Phin-
ney, 1989, 1993) holds that a more secure ethnic or
racial identity is associated with greater acceptance
of other groups. As individuals become more confi-
dent of their own group membership, they are as-
sumed to be more open to other groups. The multi-
culturalism hypothesis (Berry, 1984) and research
based on this hypothesis (Berry, 1984; Berry, Kalin, &
Taylor, 1977; Lambert, Mermigis, & Taylor, 1986) also
indicate that individuals with a positive and secure
sense of their own culture will have positive attitudes
toward other groups. In research with adults in Can-
ada, Berry et al. (1977) found that those ethnic groups
with a more secure group identity had more positive
intergroup attitudes. Lambert et al. (1986) found that
cultural security correlated strongly with favorable
attributions of other groups.
In the present study, on the basis of both multicul-
turalism and developmental views, we expected that
adolescents with higher ethnic identity scores, indica-
This content downloaded from
128.54.203.194 on Sun, 29 Oct 2023 23:04:57 +00:00
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms